KU Center for Research on Learning

KU Center for Research on Learning

Images ok bike riders from the STRUCTURE Your Reading book cover representing the 3 steps in the strategy.



Results of a Two-Year Study on the Effectiveness of STRUCTURE Your Reading

Download this profile (PDF)

Dr. Barbara J. Ehren, University of Central Florida


Conclusions

  1. After one year of instruction in the SYR strateroutine, treatment groups at all grades demonstrated significant gains in either (or in sixth grade both) the asking of self-prompting questions or the use of strategies before, during, and after reading. However, no significant gains were made on the standardized reading measure (DRP). A plausible explanation is that learning metacognitive behaviors such as self-questioning and strategy use may take more time to generalize to standardized testing than the dosage (14-39 hrs.) facilitated. An equally plausible explanation is that the format of the DRP did not lend itself to the use of the SYR packaging strategy, which was designed to promote strategic reading in authentic classroom reading tasks.

  2. In Year One, the sixth-grade control group out-performed the treatment group, albeit not significantly and with a small effect size. Although the sixth-grade treatment group did significantly better in self-questioning and strategy use, fidelity checks revealed sporadic attention to overall reading comprehension with a low dosage (14 hrs.) of SYR instruction in the treatment class.

  3. The most dramatic results were seen in Year Two with eighth-grade students in the treatment group, who made significant gains with large to very large effect sizes on the standardized reading comprehension measure as well as self-questioning and strategy use. It is reasonable to suspect that maturation with these older students may enhance their ability to engage in metacognitive tasks and apply metacognitive behaviors to a broad array of reading tasks, including standardized tests.

  4. In Year Two, neither sixth- nor seventh-grade adolescents in treatment groups had significant increases in reading comprehension scores, with only seventh-graders who had received SYR instruction for two years making significant gains in metacognitive behaviors with medium to large effect size (.75) in self-questioning, even with higher dosages in Year Two (sixth=68; seventh=90). Interpretations proffered in #1 may apply. Additionally, fidelity may be an issue; a more detailed approach to fidelity checks is warranted in future research.

  5. Both high and low achievers appear to benefit from SYR instruction in terms of metacognitive behaviors and reading comprehension but more so after two years of instruction with larger effects for the low achievers. Both favored self-questioning.

  6. SPED students and the LD subset showed gains in strategies after Year One, with large effects for the LD group. The LD group also showed gains in self-questioning with medium effect size. After Year Two, only the SPED group as a whole showed significant gains in use of strategies with a large effect.

  7. The SYR instructional protocol calls for equal attention to the self-questioning prompts and strategic actions associated with each step of the strateroutine. It is therefore interesting to note that in some instances, the adolescents evidenced better use of one over the other. It is possible that teacher emphasis on one could account for those results. It is also possible that students may favor the use of one metacognitive behavior over the other. Future research might explicate the relationship of self-questioning and strategic action in metacognitive reading tasks.



References

  • Baker, L., & Brown, A.L. (1984). Metacognitive skills and reading. Handbook of Reading Research, 3, 353-394.


  • Borkowski, J.G. (1992). Metacognitive theory: A framework for teaching literacy, writing, and math skills. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25(4), 253-257.


  • Deshler, D.D. & Schumaker, J.B. (1986). Learning strategies: An instructional alternative for low-achieving adolescents. Exceptional Children, 52(6), 583-590.


  • Duffy, G.G. & Roehler, L.R. (1989). Why strategy instruction is so difficult and what we need to do about it. In C.B. McCormick, G. Miller & M. Pressley (Eds.) Cognitive strategy research: From basic research to educational applications. New York: Spriger-Verlag.


  • Edmonds, M., Vaughn, S., Wexler, J., Reutebuch, C., Cable, A., Tackett, K., & Schnakenberg, J. (2009). A synthesis of reading interventions and effects on reading comprehension outcomes for older struggling readers. Review of Educational Research, 79(1), 262–300.


  • Gersten, R., Fuchs, L.S., Williams, J.P., & Baker, S. (2001). Teaching reading comprehension strategies to students with learning disabilities: A review of the research. Review of Educational Research, 71(2), 279–320.


  • Kamil, M.L., Borman, G.D., Dole, J., Kral, C.C., Salinger, T., and Torgesen, J. (2008). Improving adolescent literacy: Effective classroom and intervention practices: A Practice Guide (NCEE #2008-4027). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Retrieved June 23, 2009.


  • Mastropieri, M.A., & Scruggs, T.E. (1997). Best practices in promoting reading comprehension in students with learning disabilities: 1976 to 1996. Remedial and Special Education, 18(4), 197-213


  • National Reading Panel (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction: Reports of the subgroups. Washington, DC: National Institute of Child Health and Development.


  • Palincsar, A.S. (1986). The role of dialogue in providing scaffolded instruction. Educational Psychologist, 21, 73-98.


  • Pressley, G.M., & McCormick, C.B. (1995). Strategies and metacognitive regulation of strategies: Basic theory and research. In Advanced educational psychology for educators, researchers, and policymakers (pp. 27-47). New York: Harper Collins College Publishers.


  • Pressley, G.M., Borkowski, J.G., & Schneider, W. (1987). Cognitive strategies: Good strategy users coordinate metacognition & knowledge. Annals of Child Development, 4, 89-129.


  • Pressley, G.M., Brown, R., Beard El-Dinary, P., & Afflerbach, P. (1995). The comprehension instruction that students need: Instruction fostering constructively responsive reading. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 10(4), 215-224.


  • Scammacca, N., Roberts, G., Vaughn. S., Edmonds, M., Wexler, J., Reutebuch, C.K., & Torgesen, J.K. (2007), Interventions for adolescent struggling readers: A meta-analysis with implications for practice. Portsmouth, NH: RMC Research Corporation, Center on Instruction.


  • Torgesen, J.K., Houston, D.D., Rissman, L.M., Decker, S.M., Roberts, G., Vaughn, S., Wexler, J., Francis, D.J., Rivera, M.O., & Lesaux, N. (2007). Academic literacy instruction for adolescents: A guidance document from the Center on Instruction.


  • Wanzek, J., Wexler, J., Vaughn, S., & Ciullo, S. (2010). Reading interventions for struggling readers in the upper elementary grades: A synthesis of 20 years of Research. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 23(8), 889-912.





Page 3 of 3 pages for this article  < 1 2 3